Too expensive or too cheap? Effect of mismatch
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.14659/PJOEP.2014.06.02Keywords:
Prospect Theory, psycho-accounting function, mismatch effect, overestimation bias, underestimation biasAbstract
This article deals with the subjective mapping of prices. It was proposed that the relationship between the actual prices of market goods and their subjective representation can be expressed by using the psycho-accounting function postulated by Prospect Theory. 316 respondents were asked to intuitively estimate the prices for nine categories of market goods. The results confirmed the proposition. The power function was established for all categories of goods. The prices of relatively cheap goods were overestimated, which was labeled as overestimation bias. The prices of relatively expensive goods were underestimated, which was labeled as underestimation bias. The discrepancy between the psycho-accounting function and the isomorphic mapping generates the mismatch effect. The practical consequences that arise from the mismatch effect for both buyers and sellers are discussed.
References
Bernoulli, D. (1954). Exposition of a New Theory on the Measurement of Risk. Econometrica, 22(1), 23.
Doble, C. W., Falmagne, J.-C., Berg, B. G. (2003). Recasting (the near-miss to) Weber’s law. Psychological Review, 110(2), 365–375.
Falkowski, A., Tyszka, T. (2002). Psychologia zachowań konsumenckich. Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.
Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Breitkopf und Härtel.
Galanter, E. (1990). Utility functions for nonmonetary events. The American Journal of Psychology, 103(4), 449–470.
Gasparski, P. (2013). Buchalter ułomny, czyli psychologiczne zasady księgowania i liczenia pieniędzy. Warszawa: CeDeWu.
Hinrichs, J. R. (1969). Correlates of employee evaluations of pay increases. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(6), 481–489.
Jozefowiez, J., Cerutti, D. T., Staddon, J. E. R. (2006). Timescale invariance and Weber’s law in choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 32(3), 229–238.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.
Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.
Kamen, J. M., Toman, R. J. (1970). Psychophysics of prices. Journal of Marketing Research (JMR), 7(1), 27–35.
Kardes, F., Cronley, M., Cline, T. (2010). Consumer behavior. Stamford: Cengage Learning.
Kornbrot, D. E., Donnelly, M., Galanter, E. (1981). Estimates of utility function parameters from signal detection experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(2), 441–458.
Lindsay, P. H., Norman, D. A. (1977). Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. Academic Press.
Samuelson, P. A. (1977). St. Petersburg paradoxes: Defanged, dissected, and historically described. Journal of Economic Literature, 15(1), 24.
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64(3), 153–181.
Stevens, S. S. (1961). To honor Fechner and repeal his law. Science, 133, 80–86.
Stevens, S. S. (1971). Issues in psychophysical measurement. Psychological Review, 78(5), 426–450.
Takahashi, T. (2006). Time-estimation error following Weber-Fechner law may explain subadditive time-discounting. Medical Hypotheses, 67(6), 1372–1374.
Thurstone, L. L. (1929). Fechner’s law and the method of equal appearing intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 214–224.
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representation of Uncertainty. Journal of Risk & Uncertainty, 5(4), 297–323.
Urban, F. M. (1933). The Weber-Fechner law and mental measurement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(2), 221–238.
Weber, E. H. (1996). E.H. Weber on the Tactile Senses. Psychology Press.
Downloads
Published
Issue
Section
License
Authorship
The Author (Co-authors) declares that the submitted paper does not infringe on the third parties’ copyright, consents to submitting the paper to the reviewing procedure and introducing editorial changes in it, and conveys proprietary copyright to the paper free of charge to publisher for uses stipulated in paragraph 50 of the Polish Bill on Copyright and Related Rights of 4.02.1994, provided that the paper has been accepted for publication and published.
Conditions of use
The Publisher owns the proprietary copyright to all materials published in the journal. Articles are made available on the basis of non-exclusive Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 3.0 licence. The full text of the licence can be found at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/legalcode, and its accessible summary a thttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/deed.en.
Posting the text in the repository, on the author’s homepage or other website is permitted on condition that it does not entail gaining material benefits and the text is equipped with full source documentation (including the title, volume, issue and internet address of the journal).
All those interested in using the materials published in the journal for commercial purposes are kindly requested to contact the Editors.